Teaching Important Relational Skills for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Intellectual Disability Using Freely Available (GO-IRAP) Software

Review Article

Austin J Autism & Relat Disabil. 2017; 3(2): 1041.

Teaching Important Relational Skills for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Intellectual Disability Using Freely Available (GO-IRAP) Software

Murphy C¹* and Barnes-Holmes D²

¹Department of Psychology, National University of Ireland Maynooth, Ireland

²Department of Psychology, Ghent University, Belgium

*Corresponding author:Carol Murphy, Department of Psychology, National University of Ireland Maynooth, Maynooth, Ireland

Received: June 01, 2017; Accepted: August 21, 2017; Published: August 30, 2017

Abstract

The current article is a brief summary of recent research in relational responding with an emphasis on the Ghent Odysseus Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (GO-IRAP) for teaching this important skill to children with diagnosed autism. Relational responding, especially derived (emergent, untaught) relational responding is thought to be related to complex human cognition such as language and other symbolic understanding (e.g., algebra, maths). Research has indicated that fluent and flexible relational responding is correlated with higher scores on standardized ability/ IQ tests, and that even quite complex relational skills may be taught to children with autism using Multiple Exemplar Training (MET) with other behavioural principles such as positive reinforcement. The GO-IRAP is an interactive computerised teaching programme conceptualized by Professor Dermot Barnes-Holmes and colleagues, which has been made freely available to practitioners and parents. This is a teaching tool designed to assess and teach relational responding from basic nonarbitrary/ physically-based relations such as coordination (same-different), comparison (greater-lesser), opposition, temporal (before-after, hierarchy, deictic relations (I-YOU), and arbitrary relations (.50=50%; X=Y) including Derived Relational Responding (DRR; teach A is greater-than B and B is greater-than, test if child derives (untaught) B is smaller than A, C is smaller than A, A is bigger than C). The current article provides some examples of the diverse relations that can be taught, and stimuli and feedback that can be presented; notably, ongoing research with the GO-IRAP may bring further refinements.

Introduction

EIBI and Importance of Language/ Communication

Behavioural research by Hayes et al. [1], and Sidman [2] has lead to new progressive behavioural teaching applications that aim to integrate relational responding, generativity and other complex aspects of language in Early Behavioural Intensive Intervention (EIBI) for children with autism or intellectual disabilities [3-5,6] Rehfeldt & Root, 2005; [7] Rosales & Rehfeldt, 2007; [8] Kilroe, Murphy, Barnes-Holmes & Barnes-Holmes, 2013 [9] Rehfeldt & Barnes-Holmes, 2009. Practical applications using EIBI to improve educational and intellectual functioning in children with autism commenced mainly with Lovass [10] using positive reinforcement (similar to contingent reward), punishment, prompting, fading, and many other behavioural principles. Beneficial outcomes from these “Applied Behaviour Analysis” (ABA) teaching procedures were that participant children (N=19) showed higher educational achievements and higher IQ scores compared to matched peers who did not undergo ABA/EIBI; further research after an extended time period showed that benefits for participants were maintained over the longterm [11]. More recently, many impartial reports have supported ABA/EIBI as an effective treatment with a supporting evidencebase for remediating skill deficits in children with autism [12]. Skinnerian behavioural principles [13] and his functional account of language [Verbal Behavior] [14] are foundational to successful ABA treatment programmes, in which language and communication have been traditionally targeted as a priority [15]. Noted limitations, however, were that ABA largely failed to target emergent, novel speech utterances, or the generativity that is characteristic of human language [1]. This was thought to be due to the absence of a complete and comprehensive behavioural account of these more complex aspects of human language and cognition.

Relational frame theory/ Derived relational responding: A modern behavioural language theory termed Relational Frame Theory [RFT; 1] has expanded the behavioural research agenda into the more complex areas of human language. The theory encompasses phenomena such as generativity, irony, sarcasm, and humour (readers unfamiliar with RFT may find it useful to commence with Torneke’s [16] account, which is readily understood). Of fundamental importance is Derived Relational Responding (DRR), which was documented in early behavioural research investigating the stimulus equivalence phenomenon. The kernel of Stimulus Equivalence (SE) and DRR is that human language entails derived, emergent or untaught responding. For example, if language-able humans are taught that A is same-as (equivalent to) B, they can derive B as equivalent to A without being taught this bi-directional relations. If they then learn B same-as C relations, they can derive A same-as C and C same-as A relations without direct teaching. Sidman’s research [2] showed that when a boy with intellectual disability was taught to relate a written word in relation to a picture, and was taught to relate that picture to a spoken word, he derived relations between the written word and the spoken word, without the necessity of teaching. This type of responding was thought to hold much promise for establishing language repertoires involving derivation in children with ID or autism and related language deficits. In practical terms however, teaching applications involving stimulus equivalence research were slow to follow, and work in the area of SE and DRR remained mainly in the basic research literature for years if not decades.

In relatively recent behavioural history, RFT researchers have fleshed out a comprehensive account of DRR, and are continuing to build a solid body of supporting research, including translational research conducted with children with autism and or ID (e.g., demonstrated derived requesting with children with autism [3-5]; showed derived requesting in adults with severe ID [6]; and other DRR skills with individuals with ID [17,18]. Many different types of DRR were expanded upon by RFT, not only derived equivalence relations, RFT described relational “frames” of distinction, opposition, comparison, hierarchy, temporal relations, deictic relations involved in perspective-taking (e.g., I-You/Here-There/Now-Then) and many others [1]. The theory also clearly differentiated between relational responding that is arbitrary and nonarbitrary, emphasizing the importance of the former over the latter.

Arbitrary relational responding: Arbitrary relational responding in RFT refers to relations that are socially designated with no physical basis, as when the word “tree” is assigned a coordinate relation (same-as) with the object tree, or when, for example, mathematical symbols are used to represent equations. Nonarbitrary relations are based on physical characteristics, for example, two identical black cats may form an equivalence relation; cats and dogs may participate in a coordinate relation in that they belong to the category “animals” based on nonarbitrary similarities such as having four legs and fur. The said dogs and cats may also participate in a nonarbitrary relational frame of comparison; the dog in some instances being physically greater than the cat, and sometimes vice versa. In contrast, an arbitrary comparative relation may be described when a language-able human is taught that regardless of physical size, an arbitrary stimulus, coin “A”, is established as “greater than” coin “B”, and B is established as greater than C, he or she can derive without further teaching that B is less than A, C is less than A, and A is greater than C. This type of DRR is said to involve a transformation of functions via stimulus relations; the value function of coin C is transformed due to its relation with coin B and so on.

Because this type of complex responding would seem essential in language and higher cognitive functioning [1], applied behavioural researchers have begun to integrate relational responding in ABA teaching programmes in combination with more well-known behavioural such as positive teaching technologies involving positive reinforcement. For example, a comprehensive RFT-based protocol named PEAK (Promoting Emergent Advanced Knowledge; [19,20] has been designed to assess relational responding skills present in the repertoires of children with autism and or ID, with detailed instructions for subsequently teaching relational responding found absent, from basic to advanced levels. Indeed, in a relatively short space of time, the PEAK programme has garnered considerable empirical support regarding correlations with established standardized measures of cognitive ability and intelligence [21,22]. Another emergent RFT-based relational training programme is the TARPA (Training & Assessment of Relational Precursors & Abilities), which is a computer-based protocol for the assessment of relational responding [23,24].

Relational flexibility and intelligence: Correlations between relational training performance and measures of ability or intelligence accord with previous predictions of RFT theory and research; a study with typically-developing adult participants showed that rapid relational responding was correlated with higher IQ scores [25,26]. These studies showed also that flexibility in relational responding was even more important; to illustrate, the ability to relationally respond with speed with reversed contingencies was more strongly associated with higher IQ scores than speed of responding per se. An example of contingency reversals is as follows: to begin, an A> B relation is reinforced as correct; a reversed contingency means a B>A relation is reinforced as correct; a double-reversal means that A>B is again reinforced as correct. Performance of participants who responded rapidly to such contingency reversals in relational responding was found to be more strongly associated with higher IQ scores, compared to participant performance data showing rapid relational responding per se [25]. The proposition that complex relational responding is correlated with higher IQ scores has received preliminary research support also in the domain of behavioural application. An applied research study recorded IQ measures pre and post relational training with children who were educationally disadvantaged, and found that participants who had learned complex (arbitrary) relational responding showed gains on IQ measures post-training [27] (i.e., minimum one standard deviation on the full-scale Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, WISC-IVUK [28] compared to those who learned less complex relational training (e.g., more basic nonarbitrary relations that are based on physicality), whose IQ measures mostly remained stable.

Intuitively, it seems likely that teaching speed and flexibility in relational responding may be more readily facilitated via an interactive computerised teaching programme compared to one-onone table-top teaching, and there has been some research to support this assumption, conducted with children with autism [8,29]. The lure of computer software teaching programmes is that by their nature they allow for rapid and consistent teaching-trial presentation, because a teacher or instructor does not have to physically manipulate and arrange presentation and removal of an array of stimuli. In addition, they can provide consistent delivery of feedback for correct and incorrect student responding across training trials. It is also possible that the student can practice learning independently, which is advantageous in terms of student automaticity and teacher resources. (Notwithstanding these advantages, a computerised teaching programme could not and should not be considered as a replacement for instructor-led teaching, but could be seen as potentially complementary to one-on-one and other instructor-lead classroom learning, which of course should always be valued as the mainstay of educational teaching and learning).

IRAP: Implicit relational assessment procedure

The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) is a computer software programme that was designed by Professor Dermot Barnes-Holmes [30], who was co-author of RFT theory [1], and who, with his wife and colleague Dr. Yvonne Barnes-Holmes, has been to the forefront in conducting basic and translational research in RFT and DRR. Their work has also led to a practical handbook with detailed descriptions on how to design behavioural applications based on RFT and DRR, in combination with traditional behavioural teaching and intervention technology [9].

As a research tool, the IRAP has been gaining ground as a behaviourally based measure of relational responding that contributes to the research literature on implicit stereotyped responding or prejudice, in socially sensitive areas in which participants may not wish to report negative bias and indeed may be unaware that they are prone to bias. The IRAP has been used to demonstrate participant’s implicit stereotype in areas such as race, attractiveness (i.e., beautybias [31], bodyweight (thin-positive versus fat-negative [32,33], social stigma toward autism [34], race stereotype [35], sexual orientation stereotype [36], and age stereotype [37], to mention just a sample. In such procedures the IRAP tasks involve participants affirming verbal relations presented onscreen, in trial-blocks that are alternatively consistent and inconsistent with stereotype responding. The assumption is that participants will respond faster when the relations presented are in accord with relations they previously learned within the social community (e.g., thin-positive; fat-negative are relations considered to be consistent with pre-learned social stereotype within the verbal community; inconsistent trial-blocks would present these relations reversed). The IRAP response latency data for consistent and inconsistent trial-blocks are analyzed in aggregate to determine if response latency data for consistent trial-blocks were significantly shorter compared to response latency data for inconsistent trialblocks, shorter latencies for consistent trial-blocks are deemed to indicate stereotyped responding in a relevant IRAP investigation [38].

T-IRAP: Interactive computerised teaching programme

Returning to teaching relational responding using the IRAP, this computer software was readily adapted for teaching relational responding with children with diagnosed ASD, and a range of picture stimuli gleaned from websites such as Board maker and similar were inserted (T-IRAP; ‘T’ for Teacher) [8,29,40]. Kilroe et al. [8] showed that 8 participants with diagnosed ASD (boys and girls aged 8-10 years, described as ‘high-functioning’) successfully learned to use the T-IRAP, and learning outcomes for relational responding showed greater speed and accuracy with T-IRAP compared to Table-Top teaching conditions with one-on-one instruction. The relational skills targeted with both T-IRAP and TT commenced initially with nonarbitrary coordination relations (e.g., animal categories, see Figure 1), then taught opposition relations (Figure 2), followed by arbitrary coordination relations (e.g., .50 same-as &su; ½ different-from ¼, Figure 3) arbitrary comparative relations (Figure 4), and derived arbitrary comparative relations (Figure 5). This DRR procedure involved the following sequence: teach/ reinforce the relations, “X” same-as SMALL, Z opposite-to X, Z same-as P; then test for Derived Arbitrary Comparative Relations (DRR), i.e., does participant correctly select response option SAME when sample stimulus (above) is Z and comparison stimulus is BIG (below) and select OPPOSITE when Z is presented with SMALL; does participant similarly select SAME when P is presented with BIG and select OPPOSITE when P is presented with SMALL).