Research Article
Austin J Clin Ophthalmol. 2018; 5(2): 1093.
An Investigation of the Visual Function and the Effect of Colored Filters on Reading Ability of Dyslexic Children
Moghaddam HO1,2, Mohammadi Z¹, Yazdani N1,2, Sobhanirad D³, Shandiz JH1,2, Khabazkhoob M4,5 and Ehsaei A1,2*
¹Department of Optometry, School of Paramedical Sciences, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Iran
²Refractive Error Research Center, School of Paramedical Sciences, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Iran
³Department of Speech Therapy, School of Paramedical Sciences, Mashhad University of Medical, Sciences, Iran
4Noor Research Center for Ophthalmic Epidemiology, Noor Eye Hospital, Iran
5Department of Medical Surgical Nursing, School of Nursing and Midwifery, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Iran
*Corresponding author: Asieh Ehsaei, Assisstant Professor of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Department of Optometry, Mashhad University of Medical Sciences, Parastar 2, Ahmadabad Avenue, Mashhad, Iran
Received: May 01, 2018; Accepted: May 28, 2018; Published: June 04, 2018
Abstract
Purpose: The aim of the present study was to evaluate the effect of colored filters on the reading ability in dyslexic children.
Method: Twelve children with dyslexia and twelve age and gender matched non-dyslexic children between second and fifth grade were recruited. Visual acuity assessment, refraction and binocular examinations including far and near heterophoria, near point of convergence and accommodation, stereopsis, and near fusional reserves were performed for all participants. Afterwards, both the colored filters and transparent ones were used in dyslexic group. Children were asked to choose one filter which enhances the clarity or perception of the text and then the reading ability was measured with and without filters.
Results: The mean age of all subjects was 8.42±1.06 years. The results showed that fusional reserves significantly reduced in dyslexic children compared to normal individuals, although no statistically significant difference was reported in other visual functions. Moreover, it has been shown that reading ability of dyslexic children was significantly improved with colored filters.
Conclusion: The results of the present study showed that fusional reserves decreased in dyslexic children. Moreover, speed and accuracy of the reading increased significantly in dyslexic children with color filters. It would be recommended that in dyslexic children, in addition to a thorough assessment of binocular vision, using color filters is beneficial.
Keywords: Dyslexia; Binocular vision; Magnocellular system; Colored filters; Colored overlays
Introduction
Reading and writing are the most important issues for learning especially in today’s computer-based society. Any disorder or disability which affects children proficiency to read or write is classified as a learning disability and may result in negative effects on educational, emotional and either social functions [1,2]. One of the most common learning disabilities is dyslexia which is characterized with debility in reading [2-4]. It has been estimated that 3% [5] to 13.67% of school-aged children are dyslexic [6]. Considering the visual system, significant association have been found between magnocellular pathway involvement and dyslexia incidence [7,8]. The main hypothesis for this association is magnocellular theory, according to which most of children with dyslexia have defect in magnocellular pathway [9]. Moreover, it has been agreed that any disorder in eye movement could be a leading cause for dyslexia [10]. Binocular instability is the most common visual sign in dyslexic patients, which is distinguished by the low fusional reserve and vergence instability [11]. Patients with binocular instability usually suffer from blur vision, diplopia, asthenopia and visual distortion [12]. Kriss and Evan reported that visual stress, which was defined as any disability to see comfort and without distortion, is more prevalent in dyslexic children compared with normal population [13].
It has been approved that decreasing the font size, using colored filters and reducing the contrast could improve the reading ability in dyslexic patients, and relieve visual symptoms through increasing patient’s perception [1,14]. Color filters, was firstly introduced by Irlen International Association [15], who suggested applying filters as an effective remedy for visual symptoms-co-occurring dyslexia and stated that the color of the therapeutic filter is different from person to person, therefore, it may not have any effect if an appropriate color does not prescribed [16]. According to previously published studies, colored filters or overlays could improve the symptoms of visual stress and reduce the asthenopia symptoms and consequently improve the reading ability in dyslexic children [17-19]. This study is proposed to assess the effectiveness of colored filters on reading speed in dyslexic children.
Methods
Study population
Twelve dyslexic children and twelve age- and gender- matched normal individuals (age range: 7 to 10 years of age) were recruited for this study according to inclusion criteria as follows: normal mean intelligence quotient between 90 and 110 (Wechsler intelligence test), being healthy without any physical problems, parents’ consent, lack of acute vision and hearing problems and no psychological problems. Children with any ocular pathology were excluded from the study population. All patients were referred by a specialist to the dyslexia comprehensive center. It is important to keep in mind that dyslexia is not a disease or an identifiable physical condition. Rather, it is a learning style that could be assessed through a profile to show whether the child has a typical pattern of strengths and weaknesses, coupled with other assessments to rule out other possible causes of symptoms, such as vision or hearing problems.
A diagnostic assessment is the most comprehensive kind of assessment. Definitive diagnosis of dyslexia has been confirmed using psychological tests, math performance tasks, sequential memory tests, cognitive and emotional growth tests and also speech therapy examination. All tests were performed by an experienced psychologist. Patients with any history of dyscalculia, attentiondeficit/ hyperactivity disorder, attention deficit disorder, ocular pathology, and amblyopia or color vision deficient were excluded from the study. Demographic data were collected for each subject and detailed visual examinations were performed including: visual acuity, refractive error, distance and near heterophoria, positive and negative fusional reserves and depth of vision.
Ethical consideration
Informed consent of a parent/guardian was obtained after explanation of the nature and possible consequences of the study. The experiment followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Mashhad University of Medical Sciences.
Visual function assessment
Subjective and objective refractions were carried out using retinoscope (Heine Beta 200 retinoscope, HEINE Optotechnic, Germany, https://www.heine.com/) and auto refractometer (Topcon RM8800, Topcon Corporation, Japan, https://global.topcon.com/). Visual acuity was measured monocularly with the Snellen chart at a distance of 6m. The cover test was done to confirm the presence of any far or near heterophoria and simultaneous prism and cover test was performed to estimate the amount of present heterophoria.
For assessment of the near point of accommodation, the correction was cited in trial frame and normal room illumination was used. Each patient observed the target at a distance of 40cm and was instructed to report the first sustained blurred, then the measured distance from the spectacle plane was converted to dioptric power. The same procedure was repeated for assessment of the near point of convergence, while the patient was instructed to report the first diplopic image of the target. All measurements were repeated for three times and average was recorded.
Positive and negative fusional reserves were evaluated using the prism bar at 40cm. Divergence or negative ranges (Base In) were tested before convergence or positive ranges (Base Out) due to possible vergence adaptation. For each section, the patient was instructed to report the blur, break and recovery of the target while adjusting the prism power.
Stereoacuity was measured in subjects using the Titmus circles test (Stereo Optical, Chicago, IL).
Reading ability
Reading ability was measured with the Decoding Skills Test (DST), which contains 10 experiments [20]. Reading ability was evaluated using three of sub tests including: reading words, reading non-words and chain of words. Hoseini et al [21] approved the validity and reliability of the reading tests. Total test time was 2 minutes. Each correct answer had one score and the highest score was 30. Raw scores are used to convert results into grade- level scores. Additionally, level score shows how the patient performs and if he or she could proceed to the next level. Thirteen filters with different colors and intensity, which were defined using UV Visible Spectrophotometer, were used. Table 1 shows the physical characteristic of each filter. Wilkins method was used to choose filters [22]. Reading skill was evaluated with and without filters. The current study was a blind study and information about the efficacy of filters was masked from the participants to eliminate the bias until after a trial outcome was known.
Filter
Wave length (nm)
Absorbance (Abs)
X1
432
1.661
X2
226
3.706
X5
677.9
0.877
X6
476.1
1.922
X7
228.1
0.294
X8
449.9
0.660
X9
288
2.176
X10
224
3.954
X12
566
0.532
X13
426
1.246
X14
567.9
0.726
X15
674
0.137
X16
609.9
0.640
Table 1: Filters physical characteristics.
Statistical analyses
All statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS software version 11.5 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA). The normality of the quantitative variables was checked using the Kolmogorov Smirnov test. Mann-Whitney U test, Wilcoxon and Friedman tests were conducted to check the efficacy of filters and comparing the means. The level of significance was set at 0.05.
Results
Twenty-four children (12 dyslexic children and 12 normal children) were included in this study. Mean age of participants was 8.42±1.16 years in dyslexic group and 8.42±0.99 years in control group.
Visual factors
Comparing dyslexic and normal children, no significant differences were noted with regards to refractive errors, visual acuity, visual depth perception, distance and near heterophoria, near point of accommodation and near point of convergence (Table 2).
Factor
Normal
Dyslexia
Total
P-value
Refractive error OD
Refractive error OS
0.66±0.45
0.68±0.54
0.58±0.60
0.60±0.56
0.62±0.52
0.64±0.54
0.64
0.68
Visual acuity OD
Visual acuity OS
9.92±0.28
9.92±0.28
9.83±0.38
9.83±0.38
9.88±0.33
9.88±0.33
0.54
0.54
Visual depth perception
40.00±0.00
40.83±2.88
40.42±2.04
0.31
Distance heterophoria
3.17±3.56
0.83±1.58
2.00±2.94
0.06
Near heterophoria
3.00±2.76
4.83±3.01
3.92±2.97
0.19
Near point of accommodation
8.25±1.28
10.33±4.18
9.29±3.21
0.46
Near point of convergence
6.08±1.16
8.25±4.02
7.17±3.10
0.42
Table 2: Means and standard deviations of visual factors in dyslexic and normal groups.
There was a statistically significant difference between two groups in fusional reserves (Table 3); the mean of dyslexic children was significantly lower than the mean value of normal children (p<0.001). The values of positive fusional reserves for break and recovery fell outside the normal range in dyslexic children, while in negative fusional reserves the minimum amounts of break and recovery ranges were similar to the normal values.
Factor
Normal
Dyslexia
Total
P-value
B.I FusionalVergence
16.50±1.24
14.00±2.82
15.36±2.42
0.02
B.O FusionalVergence
24.50±2.71
15.00±2.86
20.18±5.55
P<0.001
B.I Recovery
13.83±1.33
10.00±2.30
12.09±2.65
P<0.001
B.O Recovery
21.83±2.48
11.00±3.01
16.91±6.13
P<0.001
Table 3: Mean of binocular vision in dyslexic and normal groups.
Colored filters
Five filters of thirteen exciting filters were chosen, which included: X1 (yellow filter), X8 (yellow filter), X13 (red filter), X2 (transparent filter) and X15 (transparent filter). The most chosen filters were X2 (41.7%) and X1 (33.3%). The remaining three filters were chosen just once (8.3%).
Reading speed
Assessing the reading ability of dyslexic children, four factors were evaluated with and without filters. The factors included: total read words, correct read words, raw score and level score. There was a statistically significant difference in mean value for four reading ability factors with and without filter (Table 4).
Test
Without filter
With filter
P-value
Reading test (correct words)
88.08±23.01
92.25±21.05
0.04
Reading test (raw scale)
88.08±23.01
92.25±21.05
0.04
Reading test (level score)
74.08±22.25
79.75±19.04
0.03
Total score
252.50±33.10
262.50±29.53
0.04
Table 4: Reading ability with and without filter in dyslexic children.
One common criteria to assess the efficacy of colored filter is an improvement of 5% in reading ability which is defined as a cut-off point [17]. Therefore, to evaluate filters efficacy, the mean values of three tests with and without filters were compared, and percentage change was obtained (Table 5).
Test
Mean without filter
Mean with filter
Efficacy percentage
Reading words (level score)
74.08
79.75
5.67%
Reading non-words (level score)
81.41
84.33
2.92%
Chain of words (level score)
88.80
92.70
3.90%
Table 5: Colored filter efficacy.
For further study of efficacy, filters were categorized into two groups: 1. Colored filters (X1, X8 and X13) 2. Transparent filters (X2 and X15). The difference between these two groups is shown in Table 6.
Colored filters (M±SD)
Transparent filters (M±SD)
p-value
Test
Without F
With F
Imp
Without F
With F
Imp
Reading words (L.V)
65.66±25.74
74.33±23.81
8.67%
82.50±16.03
85.16±12.65
2.66%
0.0036
Reading non-words
(L.V)
77.33±28.94
78.83±22.24
1.5%
85.50±10.85
89.83±13.87
4.33%
0.36
Chain of words (L.V)
88.20±7.85
90.80±7.49
2.6%
89.40±8.08
94.60±11.84
5.2%
0.12
F: Filter; Imp: Improvement; L.V: Level Score; M: Mean; SD: Standard Deviation.
Table 6: Effect of colored and transparent filters on reading ability.
Based on our results the colored filters could significantly (8.67%) improve the word reading ability in comparison with transparent filters.
Discussion
This study examined the efficacy of colored filters on reading ability of dyslexic children. The author’s attention concentrated not only on color filter effectiveness, but also on visual function in dyslexic children. This study showed that fusional reserves in dyslexic children were significantly lower than normal group. These results are in good agreement with Evans and Kapoula experiments [23,24]. Adequate vergence is fundamental for maintaining the visual direction and also is a basic requirement for reading. According to Jainta and Kirkby experiments, dyslexic children suffer from binocular instability and fixation disparity while reading [11,25]. Evaluation of fusional reserve is the most appropriate test to assess the binocular instability in dyslexic patients [26]. This test can be performed at any distances using prism bar, however, as reading is usually more relevant with near vision measurement, test is performed at close distance in dyslexic children. Fusional reserve training would simply improve binocular instability. The present results are consistent with results of Evans study [27], which found no correlation between visual factors and reading ability, indeed the deficiency in reading ability is not relevant to visual disorders. Our results showed that filters remarkably improved the reading skill by 5.67%. Red and yellow filters could improve the reading ability by 8.67%. The results further indicated that transparent filters improved the chain of word reading ability (5.2%) than the colored filters. Although the improvement was not statistically significant, but clinically it reached the significant level and cause a 5.2% (cut off point 5%) improvement. Our results support the hypothesis that colored filters are more profitable for those patients with extensive reading disability [17]. The results of our study concur with Wilkins, Bouldoukian, Northway, Ray and Lightstone studies, that colored filters are more effective in improving reading ability [17-19,28-30]. It should be noted that previous studies had some limitations, for example in most of these studies, sampling is biased and pre-determined [17,18], and patients had previously used filters and declared that filters had improved their ability. Ritchie [31] resolved this issue; however, his study included all patients with different types of learning disorders. In our study, these two limitations have been resolved and only the dyslexic children were participated with no history of filter usage. For this purpose, all the subjects underwent a comprehensive examination with and without filters. Although, this research was carefully prepared, there are some limitations. First, due to low cooperation of the dyslexic children few dyslexic children recruited in the study and second, accommodative tests are limited. Further study with concentration on accommodative disorders would be of interest.
Conclusion
Finally, it can be concluded that dyslexic children do not have considerable difference in refractive errors, visual acuity, visual depth perception, distance and near heterophoria, near point of accommodation and near point of convergence in comparison with normal children, except for fusional vergence which was significantly reduced in dyslexic children. Moreover this paper has clearly shown that applying filters could improve the reading ability in dyslexic patients and improved their skills.
Acknowledgment
The authors would like to offer their sincere gratitude to the Research assistant of Mashhad University of Medical Sciences for their financial support that made this study (research code: 930336) possible. They also offer their sincere gratitude to Dr. Maerozi, Miss Kadkhodaei and Miss Tabe for their support.
References
- Hall R, Ray N, Harries P, Stein J. A comparison of two-coloured filter systems for treating visual reading difficulties. Disabil Rehabil. 2013; 35: 2221-2226.
- Dhanda A, Tushar J. Prevalence and Patterns of Learning Disabilities in School Children. Delhi Psychiatric Journal. 2013; 16.
- Billard C, Delteil-Pinton F. Dyslexia: clinical characteristics. Arch Pediatr. 2010; 17: 1734-1743.
- Shaywitz BA, Fletcher JM, Shaiwitz SE. Defining and classifying learning disabilities and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Neurol. 1995; 10: 550-557.
- Sun Z, Zou L, Zhang J, Mo S, Shao S, Zhong R. Prevalence and associated risk factors of dyslexic children in a middle-sized city of China: a cross-sectional study. PLoS One. 2013; 8: e56688.
- Rao S, Raj A, Ramanathan M, Sharma A, Dhar M, Thatkar P. Prevalence of dyslexia among school children in Mysore. International Journal of Medical Science and Public Health. 2016; 6: 159-164.
- Chase C, Ashourzadeh A, Kelly C, Monfette S, Kristofer K. Can the magnocellular pathway read? Evidence from studies of color. Vision Res. 2003; 43: 1211-1222.
- Stein J. The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2001; 7: 12-36.
- Ramus F, Rosen S, Dakin SC, Day BL, Castellote JM, White S, et al. Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain. 2003; 126: 841-865.
- Pavlidis GT. Eye movements in dyslexia: their diagnostic significance. J Learn Disabil. 1985; 18: 42-50.
- Jainta S, Kapoula Z. Dyslexic children are confronted with unstable binocular fixation while reading. PLoS One. 2011; 6: e18694.
- Alanazi MA, Alanazi SA, Osuagwu UL. Evaluation of visual stress symptoms in age-matched dyslexic, Meares-Irlen syndrome and normal adults. Int J Ophthalmol. 2016; 9: 617-624.
- Kriss I, Evans BJW. The relationship between dyslexia and Meares-Irlen Syndrome. Journal of Research in Reading. 2005; 28: 350-364.
- Evans BJ, Wilkins AJ, Brown J, Busby A, Wingfield A, Jeanes R, et al. A preliminary investigation into the aetiology of Meares-Irlen syndrome. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1996; 16: 286-296.
- Irlen H. Reading By The Colors Overcoming Dyslexia and Other Reading Disabilities Through the Irlen Method. 1991.
- Irlen H. The Irlen Revolution: A Guide to Changing Your Perception and Your Life. 2010.
- Wilkins AJ. Colored overlays and their effects on reading speed: a review. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2012; 22: 448-454.
- Bouldoukian J, Wilkins AJ, Evans BJ. Randomised controlled trial of the effect of coloured overlays on the rate of reading of people with specific learning difficulties. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2002; 22: 55-60.
- Northway N, Manahilov V. Coloured filters improve exclusion of perceptual noise in visually symptomatic dyslexics. Journal of Research in Reading. 2010; 33: 223–230.
- Richardson E, DiBenedetto B, Adler A. Use of the decoding skills test to study differences between good and poor readers. 1982.
- Hosaini M, Moradi A, Nouri R, Hassani J, Parhoon H. Reliability and Validity of Reading and Dyslexia Test. Advances in Cognitive Sceince. 2016; 18: 22-24.
- Wilkins AJ. Reading Through Colour: How Coloured Filters Can Reduce Reading Difficulty, Eye Strain, and Headaches. 2003.
- Evans BJ, Drasdo N, Richards IL. Investigation of accommodative and binocular function in dyslexia. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1994; 14: 5-19.
- Kapoula Z, Bucci MP, Jurion F, Ayoun J, Afkhami F, Bremond-Gignac D. Evidence for frequent divergence impairment in French dyslexic children: deficit of convergence relaxation or of divergence per se? Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2007; 245: 931-936.
- Kirkby JA, Blythe HI, Drieghe D, Liversedge SP. Reading text increases binocular disparity in dyslexic children. PLoS One. 2011; 6: e27105.
- Wajuihian SO, Naidoo KS. Visual factors and dyslexia: A research review. S Afr Optom. 2010; 69: 58-68.
- Evans BJ. Visual Factors in Dyslexia. The Study of Dyslexia (pp. 1-22): Springer. 2004.
- Lightstone A, Lightstone T, Wilkins A. Both coloured overlays and coloured lenses can improve reading fluency, but their optimal chromaticities differ. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1999; 19: 279-285.
- Wilkins AJ, Sihra N, Myers A. Increasing reading speed by using colours: issues concerning reliability and specificity, and their theoretical and practical implications. Perception. 2005; 34: 109-120.
- Ray NJ, Fowler S, Stein JF. Yellow filters can improve magnocellular function: motion sensitivity, convergence, accommodation, and reading. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2005; 1039: 283-293.
- Ritchie SJ. A comparison of two-coloured filters systems for treating visual reading difficulties. 2010.