Research Article
Ann Depress Anxiety. 2014;1(7): 1031.
Maternal Anxiety During and After Pregnancy: Effects on Child Internalizing Behaviour
Sue K Adams1*, Jennifer F. Daly1 and Kathleen N Morgan2
1Department of Human Development and Family Studies, University of Rhode Island, USA
2Department of Psychology, Wheaton College (MA), USA
*Corresponding author: Sue K Adams, Department of Human Development and Family Studies, University of Rhode Island, Transition Center 208, 2 Lower College Road, Kingston, RI 02881USA
Received: September 30, 2014; Accepted: November 03, 2014; Published: November 14, 2014
Abstract
Background: Anxiety during pregnancy can have significant effects on both a developing fetus and long-term developmental outcomes. In animal and human models, prenatal anxiety has been associated with characteristics of inhibited temperament in infants, which may be a precursor to later anxiety disorders. This pilot study examined if maternal report of anxiety during pregnancy differentially explained preschool-aged children’s internalizing behaviour over and above a mother’s trait anxiety. In addition, using multi-reporter methodology, the study examined if maternal anxiety during pregnancy predicted multiple reporters’ ratings of child internalizing behaviour.
Method: Participants in this pilot study included preschool-aged children, their mothers, and teachers. Retrospective maternal anxiety during pregnancy, current maternal anxiety and multi-reporter ratings of children’s internalizing behaviour were assessed.
Results: In a sample of 46 mothers, their preschool children, and the children’s teachers, hierarchical multiple regression analyses indicated that after controlling for a mother’s current level of anxiety, maternal anxiety during pregnancy significantly predicted children’s internalizing behaviour as reported by their mothers, but not as reported by teachers or other observers.
Conclusion: The prenatal environment may have a significant effect on child internalizing over time. Findings also suggest that mothers may be the most reliable reporters of child behaviour because of an overarching knowledge of the child’s behaviour across contexts.
Introduction
Pregnancy is often perceived as an exciting milestone in a woman’s life. For many women, however, it is also a time wrought with concerns about their child’s and family’s health and well-being [1,2]. In fact, expectant mothers appear to be especially vulnerable to anxiety [3,4]. Occurrences of clinical-level anxiety in this population are found to be high when compared to rate estimates of anxiety in non-pregnant fertile women [4], with as many as 54% of pregnant women reporting elevated levels of anxiety in at least one of three prenatal assessments [3].
Anxiety during pregnancy is associated with a number of prenatal and perinatal complications, the putative mechanism for which is excessive cortisol secretion in the mother and disruption of the developing hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the fetus [5]. The resulting consequences can include an increased risk for lower birth weights [6] more frequent premature delivery [7,8], and less optimal performance on the Brazelton Neonatal Behaviour Assessment Scale [9]. In addition, prenatal anxiety can alter the developmental trajectory of infants and children, resulting in children that are more irritable or difficult to soothe [10,11]. Children of mothers who were gestationally stressed have also been shown to exhibit behaviourally inhibited temperaments [12] that increase their risk for anxiety disorders later in life [13,14]. Given the high prevalence of anxiety disorders during pregnancy, exploring the short- and long-term effects of prenatal anxiety on the developing fetus is judicious.
The role of the prenatal and postnatal environments on development
Studies of prenatal anxiety and perinatal fetal outcomes in nonhuman animal models have the advantage of a level of control that is not easily duplicated in human studies. In these studies, researchers have identified a causal relation between prenatal stress and behavioural effects in nonhuman primate and rodent infants including behavioural inhibition when placed in social situations [15], impaired motor skills [16], reduced exploratory behaviour [17], and reduced social play [18]. Clarke and Schneider [19] examined the effects of prenatal stress on the postnatal stress responses of juvenile rhesus monkeys and found that prenatally-stressed subjects exhibited less normal social behaviour (i.e., proximity, contact) and more abnormal social behaviour (i.e., mutual clinging) than controls, suggesting that exposure to stress in utero may have a long-term effect on offspring behavioural reactivity, producing offspring with an intensified response to stress.
Correlational studies in human populations have yielded similar results. For instance, Coplan, O’Neil, and Arbeau [12] investigated whether maternal trait and state anxiety during pregnancy would differentially predict temperament in human infants. Researchers found that antenatal state anxiety measured during the third trimester of pregnancy predicted less positive infant affect and a lower attention span at three months of age. High levels of cortisol in pregnant women at 30-32 weeks gestation has also been shown to be significantly related to greater infant negative reactivity at 2 months old and predictive of infant temperament as assessed by a measure of infant negative reactivity [20].
It is also important to note that the postnatal environment may play a significant role in the transmission and development of anxious temperament in infants, children, and adolescents. For instance, the process of breastfeeding transmits glucocorticoids, such as cortisol, from mother to infant [21]. Breastfed infants whose mothers had elevated cortisol levels have demonstrated increased fearful behaviour and negative temperaments [21,22], and these patterns are not found in formula-fed infants [22]. Additionally, with regard to the link between behavioural inhibition and anxiety disorders later in life, Chronis-Tuscano and colleagues [13] conducted a longitudinal study examining temperament and anxiety in 126 infants and their mothers. Results revealed that mothers who reported stable and consistent behavioural inhibition over their children’s involvement in the study had a significantly higher likelihood of having adolescents diagnosed with anxiety disorders [13]. Not only does anxiety interfere with a child’s social and emotional development, but it is the most prevalent psychiatric disorder among children and adolescents [23] and often persists into adulthood causing individuals significant impairment across multiple domains of life [24,25].
Issues related to single and multi-method reporting
A limitation of many studies is the issue of single reporter bias [26], a methodological concern that arises in numerous empirical investigations of children’s behaviour. The problematic nature of single reporter designs is grounded in a substantial literature indicating that social and emotional behaviours, such as internalizing, frequently exhibit variation across situational contexts [27]. One methodological approach to remedy this dilemma has been the use of multiple reporters to assess the target behaviour. However, while the intuitive benefits of a multiple reporter approach are easily grasped, the use of multiple reporters to assess social and emotional behaviours introduces its own unique set of problems. Assessments of target behaviour often differ across reporters [28], and research has yet to resolve the debate as to which individuals (e.g. parent, teacher, trained observers or child) are the most accurate reporters of child anxiety [29].
In a seminal, meta-analytic study, Achenbach and colleagues [28] investigated the consistency of multiple reporters of behavioural and emotional problems of children and adolescents. While they found fairly high correlations among similar reporters (e.g., parent pairs), the mean correlation among different reporters was low (e.g., parent/teacher). The investigators report that each class of reporter differentially contributes to the variance of target variables, arguing that target variables differ across contexts, rather than that certain types of reporters offer less valid or reliable reports. Yet others have suggested that parents are the most reliable reporters of externalizing behaviours and children the most reliable reporters of internalizing behaviours [27]. In young children, however, it has been hypothesized that children are more likely to share worries and concerns with their parents, resulting in parents as the most reliable reporter of internalizing symptoms [30]. Further exploration is warranted to determine the specific nature and contribution of the different reporters (e.g., parents, teachers, objective observers) on child internalizing behaviours.
The current study
The purpose of this study was two-fold. First, we aimed to examine if maternal report of anxiety during pregnancy differentially explains preschool-aged children’s internalizing behaviour over and above a mother’s trait anxiety. Second, using multi-reporter methodology, we aimed to examine if maternal anxiety during pregnancy would predict multiple reporters’ perceptions of child internalizing behaviour. It was hypothesized that after controlling for a mother’s current level of anxiety, maternal anxiety during pregnancy would significantly predict children’s internalizing behaviour as reported by the mother, teacher, and observer ratings (Figure 1).
Figure 1: Study model.
Method
Design and recruitment
Data for this pilot study were collected from mothers with children enrolled in a college affiliated nursery school and a local government sponsored preschool, Head Start, which provides comprehensive early childhood services to low-income families. Both schools were located in the same rural town in the Northeast United States. The children’s preschool lead teachers also participated in the study. Parents and teachers were asked to complete various questionnaires that assessed the primary study variables. Second, observations of children’s behaviour in their preschool class were conducted via an observation room. Participation in the study was completely voluntary. The authors strictly adhered to the methodology approved by the institution’s Institutional Review Board.
Study instruments
All participants were asked to complete a demographic questionnaire. The following measures assessed primary study variables.
State trait anxiety inventory (STAI): The STAI consists of 20 questions assessing state anxiety and 20 questions assessing trait anxiety. Some sample state questions included “I feel calm, I am worried, and I am jittery.” Sample trait questions included “I felt calm, cool and collected, and I felt inadequate.” In our sample, reliability for the state anxiety measurement was α=.93, and reliability for trait anxiety was also a=.93. Two questions in the State portion were modified to ask how a mother generally felt during her pregnancy, and there is support for the reliability of a modified STAI [31,32].
Child behaviour checklist (CBCL): Mothers were asked to rate their children on the Internalizing Behaviors subscale of the CBCL, which includes items such as emotional reactivity, anxiety/depression, somatic complaints, and withdrawal [28]. Sample questions included “(my child) doesn’t answer when people talk to him/her, gets too upset when separated from parents, and is too fearful or anxious.” The CBCL subscale was reliable, a=.86, for this sample.
Data analysis
SPSS 19.0 was used to generate descriptive analyses exploring demographic characteristics of the sample. In all analyses, a P-value of less than .05 was considered significant. Pearson product-moment correlational analyses and t-tests were conducted to examine the association between demographic/descriptive, predictor and outcome variables. Hierarchical regression analyses assessed multivariate associations among primary study variables.
Results
Participants included 46 preschool-aged children, their mothers, and teachers. Children’s ages ranged from 3 ½ to 5 years old. Fiftynine percent of the children were female and mostly White (non- Hispanic). The average socioeconomic status across families was between $61,400 and $128,100 (Table 1). No significant demographic differences emerged between children attending the college-affiliated nursery school and Head Start.
Variable
% of sample
Mean
SD
Range
Sex
Female
58.7% (n=27)
Male
41.3% (n=19)
Birth weight
7.590
1.5283
3.2-10.6
Child’s birth order
1.89
.823
1-4
1
37.0% (n=17)
2
39.1% (n=18)
3
21.7% (n=10)
4
2.2% (n=1)
Full term
Yes
89.1% (n=41)
No
10.9% (n=5)
Mom delivery age
32
4.006
18-39
Married (at delivery)
Yes
91.3% (n=42)
No
8.7% (n=4)
Married (now)
Yes
84.8% (n=39)
No
10.9% (n=5)
Did not report marital status (n=2)
Income
Less than $25,530
6.5% (n=3)
$25,350-$61,400
10.9% (n=5)
$61,400-$128,100
56.5% (n =26)
$128,100-$278,450
23.9% (n=11)
Did not report income (n=1)
Years of Education
15.83
2.036
12-22
Ethnicity
100.0% white (n=46)
Employed (at delivery)
Yes
71.7% (n=33)
No
28.3% (n=13)
Employed (now)
Yes
58.7% (n=27)
No
41.3% (n=19)
Table 1: Demographic Characteristics of Participants (n = 46).
Preliminary analyses
The preliminary analyses included tests for normality and multicollinearity. To test for normality, descriptive statistics including skewness and kurtosis were computed, and histograms were reviewed. Mean transformations were conducted when indicated. Due to multicollinearity between predictor variables (anxiety during pregnancy and trait anxiety) the method of centring the means was employed according to recommendations by Jaccard and Turrisi [34]. Means and standard deviations for key demographic and primary study variables are presented in (Table 2).
Variable
Mean
SD
Range
Kurtosis
Skewness
Maternal Anxiety during Pregnancy
36.35
10.804
23-71
1.163
1.135
Maternal Anxiety Now
33.09
10.215
20-63
1.241
1.058
Child Internalizing (Parent Report)
7.61
6.347
0-27
.939
1.194
Child Internalizing (Teacher Report)
4.09
4.330
0-17
1.602
1.39
Child Reticence
2.47
2.16
0-9.67
2.86
2.56
Table 2: Mean, Standard Deviation, Range, Kurtosis, and Skewness of Variables.
In the current sample, the average rate of maternal trait anxiety was 33.1. The average rate of retrospective report of maternal anxiety during pregnancy was 36.4. Moreover, t-tests revealed that maternal anxiety during pregnancy and trait anxiety were significantly different (t(45) = 21.97, p<.001), with mothers reporting higher anxiety during pregnancy (mean=36.35) than current anxiety (mean=33.09). Similarly, in the current sample, maternal report of children’s internalizing was significantly higher (mean=7.61) than teacher report of children’s internalizing (mean=4.09).
Correlational analyses
Pearson product moment correlations were conducted to examine associations among all demographic variables and primary study variables. All demographic variables significantly correlated with primary study variables were controlled for in multivariate analyses. Older mothers had children who were rated lower on internalizing behaviour by teachers (r = -.30, p = .04). Birth order was associated with higher anxiety during pregnancy score (r = .37, p = .01), such that first time mothers reported more anxiety during their pregnancies. Preterm labour was also related to anxiety during pregnancy (r = .38, p = .009), with mothers who had preterm babies reporting higher levels of anxiety.
Among primary study variables, maternal ratings of anxiety during pregnancy were significantly correlated with present levels of maternal anxiety (r = .54, p<.001). High maternal ratings of children’s internalizing were significantly correlated with high levels of maternal anxiety during pregnancy (r = .50, p<.001). Maternal ratings of child internalizing were also correlated with teacher ratings of shyness (r = .31, p <.05) (Table 3).
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
1. Child's birth order
1
.043
-.035
.218
-.148
-.047
.374*
.215
.017
.177
2. Mother delivery age
1
-.285
.021
-.588**
-.506**
-.101
.118
-.14
-.299*
3. Birth weight
1
.502**
.285
-.206
-.214
-.047
-.200
.048
4. Full term
1
-.108
.122
.381**
.121
.289
-.007
5. Married at delivery
1
.750**
-.053
-.186
.007
.192
6. Married (now)
1
.190
.010
-.025
.142
7. Maternal Anxiety during pregnancy
1
.544**
.495**
.103
8. Maternal anxiety now
1
.248
-.055
9. Parent report of Child Internalizing
1
.308*
10. Teacher report of Child Internalizing
1
Table 3: Intercorrelations of Demographic and Primary Study Variables (n=46).
Primary analyses
Pearson product moment correlations indicate that the demographic variables of preterm labour and higher birth order were significantly correlated with the independent variables. Therefore, preterm labour and birth order were controlled for in the first step of all regression analyses. Additionally, due to the potential multicollinearity between anxiety during pregnancy and trait anxiety, trait anxiety was entered into the second step. The results of the regression analyses are below and indicated in (Table 4).
I: Dependent variable = Maternal Report of Child Internalizing
Predictor
SE
b
R2
F
p
I. Full Term Birth
Birth Order
.475
.182
.299
-.048
.086
.086
2.013
2.013
.052
.749
II. Trait Anxiety
.014
.234
.138
2.234
.119
III. Maternal Anxiety during Pregnancy
.016
.527
.294
4.258
.004**
Note. p <.05 * p < .01 ** p < .001 ***
II: Dependent variable = Teacher Report of Child Internalizing
Predictor
SE
b
R2
F
p
I. Full Term Birth
Birth Order
.488
.187
-.048
.188
.034
.034
.749
.749
.756
.228
II. Trait Anxiety
.015
-.094
.042
.616
.546
III. Maternal Anxiety during Pregnancy
.019
.166
.058
.626
.418
III: Dependent variable = Observation of Child Reticence
Predictor
SE
b
R2
F
p
I. Full Term Birth
Birth Order
1.055
.404
-.122
-.121
.036
.036
.799
.799
.432
.436
II. Trait Anxiety
.033
.102
.038
.549
.774
III. Maternal Anxiety during Pregnancy
.041
.102
.044
.467
.619
Table 4: Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analyses (N=46).
Hierarchical regression analyses indicated that after controlling for birth order, preterm birth and trait anxiety, 1) anxiety during pregnancy significantly predicted children’s internalizing behaviour as reported by the mother (β= .53, p < .01*), 2) anxiety during pregnancy did not significantly predict children’s internalizing behaviour as reported by the teacher (β= .17, p = .42), and 3) anxiety during pregnancy did not significantly predict children’s reticent behaviour as observed by study researchers (β= .10, p = .62).
Discussion
The purpose of this pilot study was to 1) examine if maternal report of anxiety during pregnancy differentially explained preschool-aged children’s internalizing behaviour over and above a mother’s trait anxiety, and 2) use multi-reporter methodology to examine if maternal anxiety during pregnancy would predict multiple reporters’ ratings of child internalizing behaviour. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses indicated that after controlling for a mother’s current level of anxiety, maternal anxiety during pregnancy significantly predicted child internalizing behaviour as reported by the mother, but not by the teacher or observer ratings.
Taken together, our most notable finding is that maternal trait anxiety was not significantly correlated with child internalizing behaviour, but maternal anxiety during pregnancy was. One could argue that as a child ages, his or her environment becomes an increasingly important factor in developmental outcomes. And although this is true, our findings also implicate the vital role of the prenatal environment in temperamental outcomes in preschool-aged children. The biological processes that occur during the prenatal stage are an integral factor in the developmental foundations of temperament [35,36]. This study supports the potential ongoing effects of the prenatal environment on children’s temperament characteristics [37], and also lends credence to the extension of an animal-based prenatal stress model to human children [38].
With regard to examining the reliability of multiple reporters as it relates to this paradigm, the findings suggest that the association between prenatal stress and child internalizing is most robustly explained by maternal report of child internalizing. Despite issues related to common reporter variance, there are many reasons why maternal report of child internalizing may be the most accurate method of assessment as compared to teacher and observer ratings [29,39]. For instance, it is possible that over the course of their time in preschool, children developed comfort with their preschool teachers, classrooms and peers, thus demonstrating fewer symptoms of shyness. Furthermore, mothers know their children across contexts and can answer questions about their behaviours from a global perspective. Children may not exhibit as many internalizing behaviours in a comfortable and supportive classroom as compared to public places or other less familiar venues. Children may also be more likely to share worries and concerns with their parents, which could lead to higher parental rating of child internalizing behaviours [30]. Our finding revealed that mothers reported higher child internalizing scores than teachers, which supports these explanations.
Methodological interpretations for this finding are also warranted. The reliability coefficient for teacher report of internalizing was respectable at a=.80; however, this reliability coefficient was lower than that of maternal report of anxiety and may be less reliable when measuring the latent construct. It is also plausible that observer ratings of child reticence did not assess the same latent constructs as measured by the CBCL. Observer ratings of reticence were purely behavioural; and purely behavioural ratings may fail to capture the important social and cognitive dimensions of internalizing. Additionally, it is possible that anxious mothers are more likely to over report and distort their children’s symptoms [30]. Overall, however, we should be careful not to dismiss the knowledge that a mother has of her children across contexts.
Study Limitations
The retrospective design of this study is a significant and noteworthy limitation. Mothers were asked to recall their experiences and emotions from 4 years prior; however, pregnancy and the birth of a child are a very important and reminiscent time in a woman’s life, and there is precedence that mothers may be better able to remember circumstances associated with pregnancy than other life events [40]. There are also postnatal factors, such as breastfeeding, that were not assessed in the present design. Breastfeeding could play an important role in the transmission of stress hormones from mother to infant, thus affecting mood and temperament [21]. Moreover, retrospective reports of anxiety have been shown to be reliable in past studies [41,42]. Issues of multicollinearity among variables are a concern when measuring state and trait anxiety variables. Therefore, appropriate statistical methods of centring the means were employed to reduce the effects of multicollinearity. Next, this pilot study had a small sample size (n=46), limiting statistical power. All subjects were White, mostly from middle to high socioeconomic status, which may have been a protective factor against elevated levels of socioeconomic stresses during pregnancy. Homogeneity of the sample also limits generalizability of findings to diverse populations, particularly women of ethnic and racial minority status who may experience more stressors during pregnancy than middle class White women [43,44]. Finally, given the study design, we were unable to determine causality. It is possible that a child’s current level of internalizing skewed maternal memories of their own stress and anxiety during pregnancy.
Implications and Future Directions
Pregnancy is a naturally anxiety-provoking time for a mother and therefore, healthcare providers should be careful not to psychopathologize normative stress and anxiety during pregnancy. However, combining psycho education and assessment of both the physical and emotional aspects of pregnancy is an important area of intervention for pregnant women. Given the potential long-term implications of stress and anxiety on a developing fetus, healthcare providers and Obstetricians should employ regular screenings for maternal anxiety during pregnancy. In an effort to improve proximal and distal outcomes, pregnant mothers who are identified as highly anxious should be referred for therapeutic services or advised on the use of coping strategies to manage stress.
References
- Melender HL. Experiences of fears associated with pregnancy and childbirth: a study of 329 pregnant women. Birth. 2002; 29: 101-111.
- Arizmendi TG, Affonso DD. Stressful events related to pregnancy and postpartum. J Psychosom Res. 1987; 31: 743-756.
- Lee AM, Lam SK, Sze Mun Lau SM, Chong CS, Chui HW, Fong DY. Prevalence, course, and risk factors for antenatal anxiety and depression. Obstet Gynecol. 2007; 110: 1102-1112.
- Andersson L, Sundström-Poromaa I, Bixo M, Wulff M, Bondestam K, åStröm M. Point prevalence of psychiatric disorders during the second trimester of pregnancy: a population-based study. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2003; 189: 148-154.
- Weinstock M. The long-term behavioural consequences of prenatal stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008; 32: 1073-1086.
- Dancause KN, Laplante DP, Oremus C, Fraser S, Brunet A, King S. Disaster-related prenatal maternal stress influences birth outcomes: project Ice Storm. Early Hum Dev. 2011; 87: 813-820.
- Lobel M, DeVincent CJ, Kaminer A, Meyer BA. The impact of prenatal maternal stress and optimistic disposition on birth outcomes in medically high-risk women. Health Psychol. 2000; 19: 544-553.
- Tegethoff M, Greene N, Olse, J, Meyer AH, Meinlschmidt G. Maternal psychosocial adversity during pregnancy is associated with length of gestation and offspring size at birth: evidence from a population-based cohort study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2010; 72: 419-426.
- Field T, Diego M, Hernandez-Reif M, Schanberg S, Kuhn C, Yando R, Bendell D. Pregnancy anxiety and comorbid depression and anger: effects on the fetus and neonate. Depress Anxiety. 2003; 17: 140-151.
- Van den Bergh B. Maternal emotions during pregnancy and fetal and neonatal behavior. Fetal Behavior Developmental and Perinatal Aspects. 1992; 851: 157-174.
- van der Wal MF, van Eijsden M, Bonsel GJ. Stress and emotional problems during pregnancy and excessive infant crying. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2007; 28: 431-437.
- Coplan R, O’Neil K, Arbeau K. Maternal anxiety during and after pregnancy and infant temperament at three months of age. Journal of Prenatal & Perinatal Psychology & Health. 2005; 19: 199-215.
- Chronis-Tuscano A, Degnan KA, Pine DS, Perez-Edgar K, Henderson HA, Diaz Y, et al. Stable early maternal report of behavioral inhibition predicts lifetime social anxiety disorder in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009; 48: 928-935.
- Marakovitz SE, Wagmiller RL, Mian ND, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS. Lost toy? Monsters under the bed? Contributions of temperament and family factors to early internalizing problems in boys and girls. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011; 40: 233-244.
- Sapolsky RM, Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR. Inhibition of glucocorticoid secretion by the hippocampal formation in the primate. J Neurosci. 1991; 11: 3695-3704.
- Schneider ML. The effect of mild stress during pregnancy on birthweight and neuromotor maturation in rhesus monkey infants (macaca mulatta). Infant Behavior and Development. 1992; 15: 389-403.
- Estanislau C, Morato S. Prenatal stress produces more behavioral alterations than maternal separation in the elevated plus-maze and in the elevated T-maze. Behav Brain Res. 2005; 163: 70-77.
- Takahashi LK. Prenatal stress: consequences of glucocorticoids on hippocampal development and function. Int J Dev Neurosci. 1998; 16: 199-207.
- Clarke AS, Schneider ML. Prenatal stress has long-term effects on behavioral responses to stress in juvenile rhesus monkeys. Dev Psychobiol. 1993; 26: 293-304.
- Davis EP, Glynn LM, Schetter CD, Hobel C, Chicz-Demet A, Sandman CA. Prenatal exposure to maternal depression and cortisol influences infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007; 46: 737-746.
- Grey KR, Davis EP, Sandman CA, Glynn LM. Human milk cortisol is associated with infant temperament. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013; 38: 1178-1185.
- Glynn LM, Davis E, Schetter C, Chicz-DeMet A, Hobel CJ, Sandman CA. Postnatal maternal cortisol levels predict temperament in healthy breastfed infants. Early Human Development. 2007; 83: 675-681.
- Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005; 62: 617-627.
- Olatunji BO, Cisler JM, Tolin DF. Quality of life in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007; 27: 572-581.
- Rapaport MH, Clary C, Fayyad R, Endicott J. Quality-of-life impairment in depressive and anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2005; 162: 1171-1178.
- Anhalt K, Telzrow CF, Brown CL. Maternal stress and emotional status during the perinatal period and childhood adjustment. School Psychology Quarterly. 2007; 22: 74-90.
- Edelbrock C, Costello AJ, Dulcan MK, Conover NC, Kala R. Parent-child agreement on child psychiatric symptoms assessed via structured interview. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1986; 27: 181-190.
- Achenbach TM, McConaughy SH, Howell CT. Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin. 1987; 101: 213.
- Frick PJ, Barry CT, Kamphaus RW. Parent and teacher rating scales. Clinical Assessment of Child and Adolescent Personality and Behavior. 2010; 2: 141-188.
- Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS, Schwab-Stone M. Discrepancies among mother, child, and teacher reports: examining the contributions of maternal depression and anxiety. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1996; 24: 749-765.
- Tenenbaum G, Furst DM. Similarities between retrospective and actual anxiety states. J Psychol. 1985; 119: 185-190.
- Tluczek A, Henriques JB, Brown RL. Support for the reliability and validity of a six-item state anxiety scale derived from the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. J Nurs Meas. 2009; 17: 19-28.
- Coplan RJ, Rubin KH, Fox NA, Calkins SD, Stewart SL. Being alone, playing alone, and acting alone: distinguishing among reticence and passive and active solitude in young children. Child Dev. 1994; 65: 129-137.
- Jaccard J, Turrisi. Interaction Effects in Multiple Regression, 2nd edn. Sage, Thousand Oaks, California, USA. 2003.
- Sapolsky RM, Meaney MJ. Maturation of the adrenocortical stress response: Neuroendocrine control mechanisms and the stress hyporesponsive period. Brain Research Reviews. 1986; 11: 65-76.
- Zentner M, Bates JE. Child temperament: an integrative review of concepts, research programs, and measures. European Journal of Developmental Science. 2008; 2: 7-37.
- Huizink AC, de Medina PG, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK. Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictors of infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002; 41: 1078-1085.
- O'Connor TG, Ben-Shlomo Y, Heron J, Golding J, Adams D, Glover V. Prenatal anxiety predicts individual differences in cortisol in pre-adolescent children. Biol Psychiatry. 2005; 58: 211-217.
- DiBartolo PM, Grills AE. Who is best at predicting children's anxiety in response to a social evaluative task? A comparison of child, parent, and teacher reports. J Anxiety Disord. 2006; 20: 630-645.
- Smith JP, Thomas D. Remembrances of things past: test–retest reliability of retrospective migration histories. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society). 2003; 166: 23-49.
- Gloster AT, Richard DC, Himle J, Koch E, Anson H, Lokers L, Thornton J. Accuracy of retrospective memory and covariation estimation in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2008; 46: 642-655.
- Marks M, Hemsley D. Retrospective versus prospective self-rating of anxiety symptoms and cognitions. J Anxiety Disord. 1999; 13: 463-472.
- Zayas LH, Cunningham M, McKee MD, Jankowski KR. Depression and negative life events among pregnant African-American and Hispanic women. Womens Health Issues. 2002; 12: 16-22.
- Troxel WM, Matthews KA, Bromberger JT, Sutton-Tyrrell K. Chronic stress burden, discrimination, and subclinical carotid artery disease in African American and Caucasian women. Health Psychol. 2003; 22: 300-309.