IQ Affection on Theory of Mind Abilities in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Down Syndrome

Special Article: Research Article

Austin J Neurol Disord Epilepsy. 2024; 10(1): 1053.

IQ Affection on Theory of Mind Abilities in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Down Syndrome

Nazila Shojaeian*

1Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain.

*Corresponding author: Nazila ShojaeianDepartment of Clinical and Health Psychology, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain. Email: nazila.shojaeian@gmail.com

Received: May 07, 2024 Accepted: May 31, 2024 Published: June 07, 2024

Abstract

Objectives: The present study reports the results of Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPMs) and Theory of Mind (ToM) first-order tasks for individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and with Down Syndrome (DS) and for typical children. However, there are few studies that have looked at the intelligence of these groups of individuals. Our aim was to investigate the absolute and relative underestimates of Theory of Mind in participants with ASD and DS in relation to Intelligence Quotient (IQ) in two countries. METHODS: A total of 74 Iranian children whose native language was Farsi participated: 24 ASD, 24 DS, and 26 controls. Similarly, 66 Swedish children whose mother tongue was Farsi: 26 ASD, 18 DS and 22 controls aged 6 to 12 years. Matching by age, gender, and socioeconomic status was performed.

Results: Children with higher verbal IQ had absolutely better performance on: Representative Change_ Question, Smarties False Confidence, Sally and Anne False Confidence,

Keywords: Theory of Mind; Autism Spectrum Disorder; Down syndrome; Intelligence Quotient

Introduction

Cognitive abilities are the understanding of mental states about feelings, thoughts, or beliefs in others, referred to as Theory of Mind (ToM). Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) with impairments in their own social communication and behavior are known as the most common neurodevelopmental disorder [1]. Children with DS, categorized with a congenital chromosomal disorder, have lower IQ potential than average, gradually declining throughout childhood IQ [2].

Several studies specifically document the IQ average in adults with DS, which ranges from 25 to 70 [3] and consider other broad research or ranges from 35 to 70 [6]. Autistic individuals are thought to have the ability to measure intellectual performance across the developmental period. While IQ is primarily unstable in childhood, all levels of IQ can occur in ASD [4,5]. To address the impairment of ToM understanding in intelligence impairment, one usually relies on studies of children with comorbidity of low IQ or psychiatric disorders such as autism or psychosis [7].

A number of studies have also demonstrated that the ASD group with higher verbal IQ scores and with higher verbal mental age can pass initial ToM tasks at their age [8]. In the study, parents and teachers of children with autism completed the Sensory Processing Measure, (GARS -2), the nonverbal Raven’s Colored task, etc. to clarify the impairment of some features such as nonverbal IQ, sensory processing and social participation that their children show at home and in the classroom [9].

To bridge to previous studies, it is worth noting that the factors verbal memory and performance IQ were documented as the best predictors of social cognitive skills in a study of three groups of children with autism disorders, a profound developmental disorder not otherwise specified, and non-autistic psychiatric disorders who were participants in the “Utrecht Department of Child Psychiatry” [10].

The Wechsler tests IQ contain some tasks to assess verbal intelligence (vocabulary and text comprehension) and performance intelligence (logical thinking and completing pictures). The verbal task of the Wechsler test is often used to assess children with autism. However, results show that comprehension difficulties in this group may reflect their weak abilities regardless of ToM performance [11]. Given the limited narrative language skills in the majority of children with autism and children with intellectual disabilities, it may be appropriate to use the non-verbal intelligence task. Nevertheless, further research is needed to better understand and know how nonverbal IQ influences the development of ToM.

While the relevance between ToM tasks and IQ in different groups of children and adults has been investigated in previous studies and received sufficient attention, foundations of the relevance between IQ and cognitive functions are needed for further research. The aim of the current study was to make a preliminary assessment of ToM performance in children with ASD and DS compared to a typical comparison group matched for age, gender and IQ. In this study, children with ASD and DS were compared to typical adults on first-order measures of ToM performance and IQ.

Tasks, Measures and Scoring

Method participants: A total of 74 Iranian preschool children participated in this study: 24 children with a formal diagnosis of ASD, 24 children with a diagnosis of DS and 26 typical children. The Swedish preschool children were similar: ASD: 26 children, DS: 18. And 22: TD participated. All these three groups matched as closely as possible in socio-economic status, gender and age. The participants were native speakers of Farsi and Swedish and all born in their country and spoke the local language. The children, aged 6 to 12 years, were tested with extended ToM tests and one test was used to assess IQ. Some children belonging to the ASD and DS groups received special education, and only a small number of these groups lived in homes in both countries. The criteria for inclusion in the study included a formal diagnosis of ASD by a psychologist or psychiatrist who is an expert in the field, a clinical diagnosis of ASD by a clinician according to the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM-5), and a language level sufficient to answer the questions on the test. In addition, for both Down syndrome (genetic diagnosis of Down syndrome) and TD: IQ > 70 and not diagnosed with a developmental or sensory impairment, the clinical records were reviewed, excluding the requirement for hearing impairment and the diagnosis of comorbid conditions.

Procedure

All tasks and procedures were structured identically in the Swedish and Iranian cultures. A single rest session, lasting 45 to 60 minutes, depends on the children’s diagnoses and conditions in each country.

Instruments

The Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPMs): The Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPMs) are a well-validated group of non-verbal tests for assessing intelligence and cognitive abilities in children and adults. It was originally developed by John C. Raven [12], whose primary goal is to study how genetic and environmental aspects influence intelligence. This test has specific items that can be tested in a group or individually. It asks for the missing item to complete a larger pattern and the correct missing item must be selected from a range of response options; it can be used at all ages. Three published versions are currently used for different groups, including 1) Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM) (for the whole age range or abilities, suitable for ages 8 to 65 years), 2) Colored Progressive Matrices (CPM) (for children aged 5 to 11 years, older people and mentally and physically impaired people and non-English speakers) and 3) Advanced Progressive Matrices (APM) (for adults with higher intelligence, such as people aged 11 years to adults). In our study, we used the same version of CPM in Iran and Sweden for the TD group under 12 years of age and for all children with specific disabilities (DS and ASD). For the typically developed children aged 12, the SPM version was used.

In each task, an answer is scored 1 as correct and 0 for an incorrect answer. Thus, there are six choices (response alternatives) in the CPM with 36 matrices evenly divided into three groups of 12 (A, AB, B) (total raw scores ranged from 0 to 36) [13, 14].The CPM consists of 60 items divided into five groups (A, B, C, D & E) of 12 items each (60 items in 5 groups of 12, and the total raw scores ranged from 0 to 60). It is important to note, however, that we used identical versions in both countries to determine children’s IQ and mental age using Raven’s progressive matrices [15]. British norms were used to assess mental age in Sweden, as there is no standardized Swedish version [16]. To standardize the Raven test in Iranian individuals, previous studies have been documented [17].

Sally and Anne task: A classic Sally and Anne task was used to assess the first order of ToM developed by Wimmer and Perner (1983) [18]. To assess children›s understanding of this task, two characters are introduced at the beginning: Sally (who has a basket) and Anne (who has a box). Sally puts her marble in a basket and then walks away. Anne then takes the marble out of the basket and puts it under the box. While Teddy Sally returned, the experimenter asked: Where does the doll girl Maryam think her marble is? (Belief question). If the children pointed to the previous place (the basket), they admitted a false belief and could pass the question. After that, «Where is the marble?» (Reality question) and «Where was the marble in the beginning?» (Memory question) were asked. The children must achieve total ToM scores between 0 and 3 [19].

Smarties tube task: A child has a tube of Smarties in front of him, which contains a pen instead of the expected Smarties, and asks two controls: “What do you think is in here? The experimenter shows the children the contents and closes the box again, then asks: When the next child comes in, what will he think is in here, does it exist if the child predicts what other people will think (Smarties or chocolate) and What was really in the box? (Pencil) [20].The total score is 0 (fail) or 1 (pass) [21].

Representational Change Test (Picture Task): This task was developed by Gopnik and Astington [22], who first presented a picture of animals whose bodies were covered except for one part (the objects resembled the versions the children had already seen, except for the last picture). Then the investigator showed the child the last picture and asked three questions: What does the child think the object is at the beginning? (Change in representation), if another child comes in (who has not seen the last object), what will he think the object is (false belief)? Also, what does the object look like and what does it really look like (distinction between appearance and reality). The total score ranges from 0 to 3 [22]. We have not found any study on the reliability and validity of the Representational Change Test.

Data Analysis

For data analysis, we used the SPSS 25 program (IBM Corp, 2013) to analyses our results. The results of the experimental measures were analyzed using the method ANOVA. The association between variables was assessed using a one-way ANOVA and a correlation coefficient. A descriptive analysis of the socio-demographic outcomes and the variables in the sample was performed, using the mean and standard deviation for the quantitative variables and the frequency and percentage for the categorical variables. Analysis of variance was used to assess differences between groups on the ToM tasks.

Results

The primary prediction was that children with a higher intelligence quotient would perform better on ToM tasks, regardless of the group to which they belonged. The data analyses for the IQ relationship to the current four subscales on all orders of Theory of Mind are presented in Table 1.